Beyond Boredom Anxiety Pdf Editor
Our central goal is to provide a definition of boredom in terms of the underlying mental processes that occur during an instance of boredom. Through the synthesis of psychodynamic, existential, arousal, and cognitive theories of boredom, we argue that boredom is universally conceptualized as “the aversive experience of wanting, but being unable, to engage in satisfying activity.” We propose to map this conceptualization onto underlying mental processes. Specifically, we propose that boredom be defined in terms of attention. That is, boredom is the aversive state that occurs when we (a) are not able to successfully engage attention with internal (e.g., thoughts or feelings) or external (e.g., environmental stimuli) information required for participating in satisfying activity, (b) are focused on the fact that we are not able to engage attention and participate in satisfying activity, and (c) attribute the cause of our aversive state to the environment. We believe that our definition of boredom fully accounts for the phenomenal experience of boredom, brings existing theories of boredom into dialogue with one another, and suggests specific directions for future research on boredom and attention.
The challenge of the task does not align with the skill of the writer, the result is a state of anxiety or boredom, both of which block the writer from entering a state of flow.
References
Bailey, J. P., Thackray, R. I., Pearl, J., Parish, T. S. (1976). Boredom and arousal: Comparison of tasks differing in visual complexity. Perceptual & Motor Skills, 43, 141–142. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Bar, M., Aminoff, E., Mason, M., Fenske, M. J. (2007). The units of thought. Hippocampus, 17, 420–428. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Bargdill, R. W. (2000). The study of life boredom. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 31, 188–219. Google Scholar Crossref |
Berlyne, D. E. (1960). Conflict, arousal and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. Google Scholar Crossref |
Bernstein, H. E. (1975). Boredom and the ready-made life. Social Research, 42, 512–537. Google Scholar ISI |
Bhana, H. (2010). Correlating boredom proneness and automation complacency in modern airline pilots. Collegiate Aviation Review, 28, 9–24. Google Scholar |
Bloomfield, L. J., Kennedy, G. (2006). Killing time: Excess free time and men’s mortality risk. In Proceedings from the Time Use and Gender Seminar (pp. 1–26). Sydney, Australia: Social Policy Research Centre, University of South Wales. Google Scholar |
Blunt, A., Pychyl, T. A. (1998). Volitional action and inaction in the lives of undergraduate students: State orientation, procrastination and proneness to boredom. Personality and Individual Differences, 24, 837–846. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Britton, A., Shipley, M. J. (2010). Bored to death? International Journal of Epidemiology, 39, 370–371. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Brown, S. W., Boltz, M. G. (2002). Attentional processes in time perception: Effects of mental workload and event structure. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 28, 600–615. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Buckner, R. L., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Schacter, D. L. (2008). The brain’s default network: Anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 1–38. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Carriere, J. S. A., Cheyne, J. A., Smilek, D. (2008). Everyday attention lapses and memory failures: The affective consequences of mindlessness. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 835–847. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Carver, C. S., White, T. L. (1994). Behavioral inhibition, behavioral activation, and affective responses to impending reward and punishment: The BIS/BAS Scales. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67, 319–333. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Chaston, A., Kingstone, A. (2004). Time estimation: The effect of cortically mediated attention. Brain and Cognition, 55, 286–289. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Cherrier, M. M., Small, G. W., Komo, S., La Rue, A. (1997). Mood state and cerebral metabolism in persons with age-associated memory impairment. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 76, 67–74. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Conrad, P. (1997). It’s boring: Notes on the meanings of boredom in everyday life. Qualitative Sociology, 20, 465–475. Google Scholar Crossref |
Critcher, C. R., Gilovich, T. (2010). Inferring attitudes from mindwandering. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36, 1255–1266. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1975). Beyond boredom and anxiety. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. Google Scholar |
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1978). Attention and the wholistic approach to behavior. In Pope, K. S., Singer, J. L. (Eds.), The stream of consciousness (pp. 335–358). New York, NY: Plenum. Google Scholar Crossref |
Csikszentmihalyi, M. (1990). Finding flow: The psychology of engagement with everyday life. New York, NY: Basic Books. Google Scholar |
Damrad-Frye, R., Laird, J. D. (1989). The experience of boredom: The role of the self-perception of attention. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 315–320. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Danckert, J. A., Allman, A. A. (2005). Time flies when you’re having fun: Temporal estimation and the experience of boredom. Brain and Cognition, 59, 236–245. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
D’Angiulli, A., LeBeau, L. S. (2002). On boredom and experimentation in humans. Ethics & Behaviour, 12, 167–176. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Davies, D. R., Parasuraman, R. (1982). The psychology of vigilance. London, England: Academic Press. Google Scholar |
Deaton, J. E., Parasuraman, R. (1993). Sensory and cognitive vigilance: Effects of age on performance and mental workload. Human Performance, 6, 71–97. Google Scholar Crossref |
De Chenne, T. K. (1988). Boredom as a clinical issue. Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 25, 71–81. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
D’Mello, S., Graesser, A. (2009). Automatic detection of learner’s affect from gross body language. Applied Artificial Intelligence, 23, 123–150. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Dumas, L. (2001). Why mistakes happen even when the stakes are high: The many dimensions of human fallibility. Medicine & Global Survival, 7, 12–19. Google Scholar |
Eastwood, J. D., Cavaliere, C., Fahlman, S. A., Eastwood, A. E. (2007). A desire for desires: Boredom and its relation to alexithymia. Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 1035–1045. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Fahlman, S. A., Mercer, K. B., Gaskovski, P., Eastwood, A. E., Eastwood, J. D. (2009). Does a lack of life meaning cause boredom? Results from psychometric, longitudinal, and experimental analyses. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 28, 307–340. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Fahlman, S. A., Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., Eastwood, J. D. (2011). Development and validation of the Multidimensional State Boredom Scale (MSBS). Assessment. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1073191111421303 Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Fenichel, O. (1951). On the psychology of boredom. In Rapaport, D. (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought: Selected sources (pp. 349–361). New York, NY: Columbia University Press. Google Scholar Crossref |
Fenichel, O. (1953). On the psychology of boredom. In Fenichel, O. (Ed.), The collected papers of Otto Fenichel (Vol. 1, pp. 292–302). New York, NY: W.W. Norton. Google Scholar |
Fenske, M. J., Raymond, J. E. (2006). Affective influences of selective attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 312–316. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Fisher, C. D. (1993). Boredom at work: A neglected concept. Human Relations, 46, 395–417. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Fisher, C. D. (1998). Effects of external and internal interruptions on boredom at work: Two studies. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 19, 503–522. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Fisher, C. D. (in press). Interest and boredom at work. In Weiss, H. M. (Ed.), Oxford handbook of work attitudes and emotions. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar |
Fraisse, P. (1963). The psychology of time. New York, NY: Harper & Row. Google Scholar |
Fraisse, P. (1984). Perception and estimation of time. Annual Review of Psychology, 35, 1–36. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Frankl, V. E. (1984). Man’s search for meaning: An introduction to logotherapy. New York, NY: Pocket Books. Google Scholar |
Freeman, F. G., Mikulka, P. J., Scerbo, M. W., Scott, L. (2004). An evaluation of an adaptive automation system using a cognitive vigilance task. Biological Psychology, 67, 283–297. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Goldberg, Y. K., Eastwood, J. D., LaGuardia, J., Danckert, J. (2011). Boredom: An emotional experience distinct from apathy, anhedonia, or depression. Journal of Social & Clinical Psychology, 30, 647–666. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Gray, J. A. (1972). The psychophysiological basis of introversion-extraversion: A modification of Eysenck’s theory. In Nebylitsyn, V. D., Gray, J. A. (Eds.), The biological bases of individual behavior (pp. 182–205). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. Google Scholar Crossref |
Greenson, R. R. (1951). Apathetic and agitated boredom. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 20, 346–347. Google Scholar |
Greenson, R. R. (1953). On boredom. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 1, 7–21. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Greicius, M. D., Krasnow, B., Reiss, A. L., Menon, V. (2003). Functional connectivity in the resting brain: A network analysis of the default mode hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100, 253–258. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Grondin, S., Macar, F. (1992). Dividing attention between temporal and nontemporal tasks: A performance operating characteristic—POC—Analysis. In Macar, F., Pouthas, V., Friedman, W. (Eds.), Time, action and cognition: Towards bridging the gap (pp. 119–128). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer. Google Scholar Crossref |
Hamilton, J. A. (1981). Attention, personality, and the self-regulation of mood: Absorbing interest and boredom. Progress in Experimental Personality Research, 10, 281–315. Google Scholar |
Hamilton, J. A., Haier, R. J., Buchsbaum, M. S. (1984). Intrinsic enjoyment and boredom coping scales: Validation with personality, evoked potential, and attention measures. Personality and Individual Differences, 5, 183–193. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Harris, M. B. (2000). Correlates and characteristics of boredom proneness and boredom. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 30, 576–598. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Hartocollis, P. (1972). Time as a dimension of affects. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 20, 92–108. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Hebb, D. O. (1966). A textbook of psychology. Philadelphia, PA: W.B. Saunders. Google Scholar |
Hicks, R. E., Brundige, R. M. (1974). Judgments of temporal duration while processing verbal and physiognomic stimuli. Acta Psychologica, 38, 447–453. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Hill, A. B., Perkins, R. E. (1985). Towards a model of boredom. British Journal of Psychology, 76, 235–240. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Hitchcock, E. M., Dember, W. N., Warm, J. S., Moroney, B. W., See, J. E. (1999). Effects of cueing and knowledge of results on workload and boredom in sustained attention. Human Factors, 41, 365–372. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Hunter, J. P., Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2003). The positive psychology of interested adolescents. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 32, 27–35. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
James, W. (1913). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Henry Holt. (Original work published 1890) Google Scholar Crossref |
Jiang, Y., Lianekhammy, J., Lawson, A., Guo, C., Lynam, D., Joseph, J. E., . . . Kelly, T. H. (2009). Brain responses to repeated visual experience among low and high sensation seekers: Role of boredom susceptibility. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 173, 100–106. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Kanevsky, L., Keighley, T. (2003). To produce or not to produce? Understanding boredom and the honor in underachievement. Roeper Review, 26, 20–28. Google Scholar Crossref |
Kant, I. (1785). Grundlegung zur Metaphysik der Sitten [Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals]. Riga, Latvia: J.F. Hartknoch. Google Scholar |
Kass, S. J., Beede, K. E., Vodanovich, S. J. (2010). Self-report measures of distractibility as correlates of simulated driving performance. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 42, 874–880. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Killingsworth, M. A., Gilbert, D. T. (2010). A wandering mind is an unhappy mind. Science, 330, 932. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Kingstone, A., Smilek, D., Eastwood, J. D. (2008). Cognitive ethology: A new approach for studying human cognition. British Journal of Psychology, 99, 317–340. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Klapp, O. E. (1986). Overload and boredom. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. Google Scholar |
Kreutzer, J. S., Seel, R. T., Gourley, E. (2001). The prevalence and symptom rates of depression after traumatic brain injury: A comprehensive examination. Brain Injury, 15, 563–576. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Kuhl, J. (1981). Motivational and functional helplessness: The moderating effect of state versus action orientation. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 40, 155–170. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Kuhl, J. (1987). Action control: The maintenance of motivational states. In Halish, F., Kuhl, J. (Eds.), Motivation, intention, and volition (pp. 279–291). Berlin, Germany: Springer. Google Scholar Crossref |
Lee, C. M., Neighbors, C., Woods, B. A. (2007). Marijuana motives: Young adults’ reasons for using marijuana. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 1384–1394. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Leech, R., Kamourieh, S., Beckmann, C. F., Sharp, D. J. (2011). Fractionating the default mode network: Distinct contributions of the ventral and dorsal posterior cingulate cortex to cognitive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(9), 3217–3224. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
LePera, N. (2011). Relationships between boredom proneness, mindfulness, anxiety, depression, and substance use. New School Psychology Bulletin, 8, 15–25. Google Scholar |
Lewinsky, H. (1943). Boredom. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 13, 147–152. Google Scholar Crossref |
Lipps, T. (1903). Leitfaden der Psychologie [Manual of psychology]. Leipzig, Germany: Wilhelm Engelman Verlag. Google Scholar |
London, H., Monello, L. (1974). Cognitive manipulation of boredom. In London, H., Nisbett, R. (Eds.), Thought and feeling (pp. 44–59). Chicago, IL: Aldine. Google Scholar |
London, H., Schubert, D. S. P., Washburn, D. (1972). Increase of autonomic arousal by boredom. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 80, 29–36. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Lundberg, U., Melin, B., Evans, G. W., Holmberg, L. (1993). Physiological deactivation after two contrasting tasks at a video display terminal: Learning vs. repetitive data entry. Ergonomics, 36, 601–611. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Maddi, S. R. (1967). The existential neurosis. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 72, 311–325. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Maddi, S. R. (1970). The search for meaning. In Arnold, W. J., Page, M. M. (Eds.), The Nebraska symposium on motivation (pp. 134–183). Lincoln: University of Nebraska Press. Google Scholar |
Maltsberger, J. T. (2000). Mansur Zaskar: A man almost bored to death. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behaviour, 30, 83–90. Google Scholar Medline ISI |
Mann, S., Robinson, A. (2009). Boredom in the lecture theatre: An investigation into the contributors, moderators, and outcomes of boredom amongst university students. British Educational Research Journal, 35, 243–258. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Martin, M., Sadlo, G., Stew, G. (2006). The phenomenon of boredom. Qualitative Research in Psychology, 3, 193–211. Google Scholar Crossref |
Mason, M. F., Norton, M. I., Van Horn, J. D., Wegner, D. M., Grafton, S. T., Macrae, C. N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315, 393–395. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
McLeod, C. R., Vodanovich, S. J. (1991). The relationship between self-actualization and boredom proneness. Journal of Social Behavior and Personality, 6, 137–146. Google Scholar |
Melton, A. M., Schulenberg, S. E. (2007). On the relationship between meaning in life and boredom proneness: Examining a logotherapy postulate. Psychological Reports, 101, 1016–1022. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Mercer, K. B., Eastwood, J. D. (2010). Is boredom associated with problem gambling behaviour? It depends on what you mean by “boredom.” International Gambling Studies, 10, 91–104. Google Scholar Crossref |
Mercer-Lynn, K. B., Flora, D. B., Fahlman, S. A., Eastwood, J. D. (2011). The measurement of boredom: Differences between existing self-report scales. Assessment. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1073191111408229 Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Mikulas, W. L., Vodanovich, S. J. (1993). The essence of boredom. Psychological Record, 43, 3–12. Google Scholar ISI |
Nakamura, J., Csikszentmihalyi, M. (2002). The concept of flow. In Snyder, C. R., Lopez, S. J. (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology (pp. 89–105). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar |
The National Center on Addiction and Substance Abuse at Columbia University . (2003). National survey of American attitudes on substance abuse VIII: Teens and parents. New York, NY: Columbia University. Google Scholar |
Newell, S. E., Harries, P., Ayers, S. (2011). Boredom proneness in a psychiatric inpatient population. International Journal of Social Psychiatry. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/0020764011408655 Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
O’Connor, D. (1967). The phenomena of boredom. Journal of Existentialism, 7, 381–399. Google Scholar Medline |
Oddy, M., Humphrey, M., Uttley, D. (1978). Subjective impairment and social recovery after closed head injury. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry, 41, 611–616. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
O’Hanlon, J. F. (1981). Boredom: Practical consequences and a theory. Acta Psychologica, 49, 53–82. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Ohsuga, M., Shimono, F., Genno, H. (2001). Assessment of phasic work stress using autonomic indices. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 211–220. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Pagnoni, G. (2012). Dynamical properties of BOLD activity from the ventral posteromedial cortex associated with meditation and attentional skills. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(15), 5242–5249. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Pattyn, N., Neyt, X., Henderickx, D., Soetens, E. (2008). Psychophysiological investigation of vigilance decrement: Boredom or cognitive fatigue? Physiology & Behavior, 93, 369–378. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Posner, J., Russell, J. A., Peterson, B. S. (2005). The circumplex model of affect: An integrative approach to affective neuroscience, cognitive development, and psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 715–734. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Posner, M. I., Petersen, S. E. (1990). The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 13, 25–42. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Posner, M. I., Rothbart, M. K. (2007). Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 1–23. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Raymond, J. E. (2009). Interactions of attention, emotion and motivation. Progress in Brain Research, 176, 293–308. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Raymond, J. E., Fenske, M. J., Tavassoli, N. T. (2003). Selective attention determines emotional responses to novel visual stimuli. Psychological Science, 14, 537–542. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Ribot, T. H. (1890). The psychology of attention. Chicago, IL: Open Court. Google Scholar |
Rogatko, T. P. (2009). The influence of flow on positive affect in college students. Journal of Happiness Studies, 10, 133–148. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Sackett, A. M., Meyvis, T., Nelson, L. D., Converse, B. A., Sackett, A. L. (2010). You’re having fun when time flies: The hedonic consequences of subjective time progression. Psychological Science, 21, 111–117. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Scerbo, M. W. (1998). What’s so boring about vigilance? In Hoffman, R. R., Sherrick, M. F., Warm, J. S. (Eds.), Viewing psychology as a whole: The integrative science of William N. Dember (pp. 135–166). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. Google Scholar Crossref |
Seel, R. T., Kreutzer, J. S. (2003). Depression assessment after traumatic brain injury: An empirically based classification method. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation, 84, 1621–1628. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Smallwood, J., Fitzgerald, A., Miles, L. K., Phillips, L. H. (2009). Shifting moods, wandering minds: Negative moods lead the mind to wander. Emotion, 9, 271–276. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Smallwood, J., O’Connor, R. C. (2011). Imprisoned by the past: Unhappy moods lead to a retrospective bias to mind wandering. Cognition & Emotion, 25(8), 1481–1490. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Smallwood, J., O’Connor, R. C., Sudbery, M. V., Obonsawin, M. C. (2007). Mind-wandering and dysphoria. Cognition & Emotion, 21, 816–842. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Smallwood, J., Schooler, J. W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132, 946–958. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Smith, R. P. (1981). Boredom: A review. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 23, 329–340. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Sommers, J., Vodanovich, S. J. (2000). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to psychological- and physical-health symptoms. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 56, 149–155. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Spreng, R. N., Mar, R., Kim, A. S. N. (2009). The common neural basis of autobiographical memory, prospection, navigation, theory of mind, and the default mode: A quantitative meta-analysis. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 489–510. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Stickney, M. I., Miltenberger, R. G. (1999). Evaluating direct and indirect measures for the functional assessment of binge eating. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 26, 195–204. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Sundberg, N. D., Latkin, C. A., Farmer, R. F., Saoud, J. (1991). Boredom in young adults: Gender and cultural comparisons. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 22, 209–223. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Thackray, R. I. (1981). The stress of boredom and monotony: A consideration of the evidence. Psychosomatic Medicine, 43, 165–167. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Thackray, R. I., Bailey, J. P., Touchstone, R. M. (1977). Physiological, subjective, and performance correlates of reported boredom and monotony while performing a simulated radar control task. In Mackie, R. R. (Ed.), Vigilance: Theory, operational performance and physiological correlates (pp. 203–216). New York, NY: Plenum. Google Scholar Crossref |
Thomas, E. A. C., Brown, T. I. (1974). Time perception and the filled-duration illusion. Perception & Psychophysics, 16, 449–458. Google Scholar Crossref |
Todman, M. (2003). Boredom and psychotic disorders: Cognitive and motivational issues. Psychiatry, 66, 146–167. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Tolor, A. (1989). Boredom as related to alienation, assertiveness, internal-external expectancy, and sleep patterns. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 45, 260–265. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Treisman, M. (1963). Temporal discrimination and the indifference interval: Implications for a model of the “internal clock”. Psychological Monographs, 77, 1–31. Google Scholar Crossref Medline |
van Tilburg, W. A. P., Igou, E. R. (2011). On boredom: Lack of challenge and meaning as distinct boredom experiences. Motivation and Emotion. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s11031-011-9234-9 Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Vodanovich, S. J., Verner, K. M., Gilbride, T. V. (1991). Boredom proneness: Its relationship to positive and negative affect. Psychological Reports, 69, 1139–1146. Google Scholar SAGE Journals ISI |
Vuilleumier, P., Driver, J. (2007). Modulation of visual processing by attention and emotion: Windows on causal interactions between human brain regions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 362, 837–855. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Wallbott, H. G. (1998). Bodily expressions of emotion. European Journal of Social Psychology, 28, 879–896. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Wangh, M. (1975). Boredom in psychoanalytic perspective. Sociological Research, 42, 538–550. Google Scholar |
Watt, J. D., Vodanovich, S. J. (1999). Boredom proneness and psychosocial development. The Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 133, 303–314. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Weber, R., Tamborini, R., Westcott-Baker, A., Kantor, B. (2009). Theorizing flow and media enjoyment as cognitive synchronization of attentional and reward networks. Communication Theory, 19, 397–422. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Weinger, M. B. (1999). Vigilance, boredom, and sleepiness. Journal of Clinical Monitoring and Computing, 15, 549–552. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Weissman, D. H., Roberts, K. C., Visscher, K. M., Woldorff, M. G. (2006). The neural bases of momentary lapses in attention. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 971–978. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
White, A. (1998). Ho hum: A phenomenology of boredom. Journal of the Society for Existential Analysis, 9, 69–81. Google Scholar |
White, R. W. (1959). Motivation reconsidered: The concept of competence. Psychological Review, 66, 297–333. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Wiesbeck, G. A., Wodarz, N., Mauerer, C., Thome, J., Jakob, F., Boening, J. (1996). Sensation seeking, alcoholism and dopamine activity. European Psychiatry, 11, 87–92. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Winkielman, P., Schwartz, N., Nowak, A. (2002). Affect and processing dynamics: Perceptual fluency enhances evaluations. In Moore, S. C., Oaksford, M. (Eds.), Emotional cognition: From brain to behaviour. Advances in Consciousness Research. (Vol. 44, pp. 111–135). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins. Google Scholar Crossref |
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition & Emotion, 24, 3–47. Google Scholar Crossref ISI |
Zakay, D. (1992). The role of attention in children’s time perception. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 355–371. Google Scholar Crossref Medline ISI |
Zuckerman, M. (1979). Sensation seeking: Beyond the optimal level of arousal. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Google Scholar |
See a Problem?
Preview — Beyond Boredom and Anxiety by Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi
Be the first to ask a question about Beyond Boredom and Anxiety
Books by University of Chicago AlumniMore lists with this book...
Depression Pdf
I have to question some of the conclusions and methodology as well. for example, the section on microflow reports research with a sample size of 20 college students providing self - reports. Surely more rigerous research is required to draw any mean...more
Sep 21, 2015
Nov 12, 2018
Jan 10, 2019
Sep 10, 2018
May 04, 2013
Dec 03, 2017
Dec 30, 2017
Beyond Boredom Anxiety Pdf Editor Pdf
He is noted for both his work in the study of happiness and creativity and also for his notoriously difficult name, i...more